By
David S. Prescott, LICSW
Many
professionals (and the lay public) came of age at a time when it was believed
that traumatized people commonly repressed all memories of having been abused. It
made sense at the time: If the abuse was bad enough, the mind could just completely
block it from memory. The mind is an amazing thing, right? The motivations were
certainly positive, with the idea that bringing abuse to light could help
people to live better lives. Those who lived through those times will recall
that these recovered memories often occurred against a backdrop of maxims such
as “always believe the victim” without questioning the methods by which people
came to believe themselves to have been victimized.
Not
long after these efforts, researchers came along and produced findings that were
nowhere near that simple. Elizabeth Loftus has become a
champion of explaining the myriad vagaries of memory, while Scott Lilienfeld
has documented a number of ways that false memories have caused life-altering harm. In a conference
presentation on hypnosis in therapy attended by the first author, Michael Yapko
once described the recovered memory movement as a near-death experience for
hypnotherapy. Havoc ensured, often referred to as the “memory wars”. Since
those days, it has been increasingly clear that people might not remember every
detail of their traumatic past, but research simply hasn’t confirmed the
complete repression/blockage of memory that has driven so many therapeutic
interventions.
Within
the past month, Henry Otgaar, Mark Howe, Lawrence Patihis and their colleagues
(including Scott O. Lilienfeld and Elizabeth Loftus) have just published a paper on the current
status of “recovering” memories of trauma that clients had long repressed. A
pre-publication version can be downloaded here. The findings are
sobering. From the abstract:
We show that the belief in repressed memories occurs on a
non-trivial-scale (58%) and appears to have increased among clinical
psychologists since the 1990s. We also demonstrate that the scientifically
controversial concept of dissociative amnesia, which we argue is a substitute
term for memory repression, has gained in popularity. Finally, we review work
on the adverse side effects of certain psychotherapeutic techniques, some of
which may be linked to the recovery of repressed memories. The memory wars have
not vanished: They have continued to endure and contribute to potentially
damaging consequences in clinical, legal, and academic contexts.
To
be clear, the authors are referring to repressed memories as the “complete
blockage” of memories related to trauma. There’s no question that our minds can
distance us from painful memories, but the standard response to trauma is more
likely to be the inability to forget about it. There is any number of
implications that follow.
First,
there is a question about the sequelae of adversity and trauma as experienced
at different ages. Under what conditions does it become unhelpful to explore
past experiences? Examples from recent publications provide points to consider.
A recent BBC article explored the
concerns of clients in treatment having to describe their abuse of others in
excruciating detail. A recent book describes, also in detail, the role of
recovered memories in the infamous Jerry Sandusky case, and the
harmful effects of treatment administered with the assumption that it was
helping. Whether one is exploring what one has done or had done to them, it is
vital to recall that treatment poorly delivered is not a benevolent event; any
problem professional can make better they can almost certainly also make worse.
Second,
whether treating abuse or preventing further harm, it can be easy to minimize
the vulnerability and suggestibility of clients in treatment. Research into coerced false confessions is just one example of how
powerful professionals can be, and how easily they can manipulate the people in
their care. Can therapists seeking to uncover past events be certain that they
are not involved in similar processes?
It
maybe worthwhile for professionals to return periodically to the codes of
ethics of their professions in order to consider the risks and benefits of the
programming they deliver and to reconsider to what extent they have the truly
informed consent of their clients. Likewise, there remains a question of what
the least restrictive alternative is: Do programs that compel complete
accountability inadvertently create new risks or cause new forms of harm that
our field hasn’t yet explored?
Ultimately, with all of the
sequelae from trauma and adversity in evidence in the lives of so many clients,
it seems there is a question of why we don’t spend more time looking forward at
constructing a better life and less time over-emphasizing the deconstruction of
the past. Important to emphasizes is that many programs are working today to
get this balance right, and many resources exist for providing treatment for
abuse and aggression that minimize the potential risks. The findings of Otgaar
et al described above are not entirely new. However, they do point to the
ongoing necessity to consider our actions. It seems that while therapeutic fads
come and go, we still face obstacles to understanding and helping our clients.
No comments:
Post a Comment