Experts have
often commented on the fallibility of memory, from Daniel Schachter’s classic
book, The Seven Sins of
Memory to Elizabeth Loftus’ TED Talk on the subject.
Most of us are aware, at least intellectually, that memory can be flawed. Unfortunately,
the discussion seems to end at the point where the fascination fades. Loftus’
showing us that we can’t identify which of the coins that are similar in
appearance is the real penny, the convincing memory of your father driving on
the right side of the road while vacationing in the UK when you were six — these
are easy for us to get our minds around.
A recent
internet cascade brought the problems of memory into a different perspective. This
time, Reddit readers got into an extended discussion of a 1990s movie called Shazaam in which the celebrity Sinbad
had played a genie. The
New Statesman produced the story, which was then picked up by other media
outlets. The opening of the article is where the implications for the field of
assessing and treating sexual violence begin:
In the early Nineties, roughly
around 1994, a now 52-year-old man named Don ordered two copies of a brand new
video for the rental store his uncle owned and he helped to run. “I had to
handle the two copies we owned dozens of times over the years,” says Don
(who wishes to give his first name only). “And I had to watch it
multiple times to look for reported damages to the tape, rewind it and check it
in, rent it out, and put the boxes out on display for rental.”
Don is
describing the movie that doesn’t actually exist. What is amazing is not the
falsity of the memories, but the extent to which to which people cling to them:
“It feels like a part of my
childhood has now been stolen from me. How does a movie simply vanish from our
history?” This isn’t Don speaking, but another man – who he has never met –
named Carl*. Carl, whose name has been changed because he wishes to
remain anonymous, recalls watching a movie called Shazaam with his
sister in the early Nineties, and has fond memories of discussing it with her
over the last 20 years.
The discussion of
this movie apparently began in 2009 on the Reddit web site and escalated to the
point where Sinbad had to comment repeatedly that he had not starred in the
movie and had never even played a genie. Still, readers cling to the belief
that he did, with confidence and vigor. This is where professionals in our
field should sit up and take notice.
People providing
treatment to those who have abused frequently accept nothing less than a
complete accounting of a person’s sexual history. In many cases, this extends
into accepting only those accounts that match the story of the person they
abused. I personally watched as entire teams of clinicians insisted that a
client in treatment should not move forward in treatment because his version had
not matched the “findings of fact” issued by the court that had convicted him. The
concern was not that he was minimizing his behavior and therefore was
participating in treatment only superficially; it was that the minutiae of his
story did not match those of the person he acknowledged he abused, as filtered
through legal documents.
Holding someone
back in treatment for the above reason is worrisome given what we know about
memory; it simply doesn’t comport with the research. Also concerning is the
confidence with which we professionals can assume that a client is actively
lying or purposefully downplaying his or her actions. It is worth asking
whether it isn’t more important to us to be confident in our certainty than it
is to accept the limitations of our knowledge. In other words, no one wants to
appear ambivalent or wishy-washy about their conclusions and opinions.
Ironically, second-rate reports overflow with confidence, while first-rate
forensic reports are written with confidence and openly acknowledge their
limitations.
It is worth
noting that many professionals in the US (and some other jurisdictions) also
rely on the polygraph to verify their clients’ accounts, despite the scientific
problems associated with it. Although polygraph
advocates rightly point out its ability to elicit more information from
clients, we still lack research to conclude that this information is always as
accurate or useful as we would like. The fact that there is still
no evidence to support the idea that polygraph examinations reduce
subsequent abuse is beyond the scope of this blog post.
My point in
raising these issues is not to discount the polygraph or the processes it
entails. Rather, it seems important to note the many ways in which “the truth”
can get lost along the way. People who are abused don’t necessarily remember
every detail correctly (we’ll come back to this). People who abuse don’t
necessarily recall every detail correctly. People who have been traumatized
frequently have problems with memory; it’s a diagnostic criterion for the
diagnosis of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. People writing the accounts of
those who abuse and those who have been abused are not without memory
fallibility either, and neither are the people who review those reports, often
recycled over the course of years through evaluations and re-evaluations.
On the other
hand, the accounts of traumatized people aren’t necessarily wrong either. Memories of abuse can be vivid and profound. There
may be no greater insult to a person who has been traumatized than to distrust their
memories. In his recent book, The
Body
Keeps the Score, trauma researcher and clinician Bessel van der Kolk discusses
these facts at length. Professionals who doubt the memories of abuse survivors
do their clients no service. After all, many aspects of the memories indeed may
be perfectly accurate, or their memory might become fragmented, with some parts
dissipating while others linger on, sometimes causing decades of distress the
person who lived them.
Adding further
insult to injury, Shane
O’Mara recently published a review of the many ways that memory fails under
high-stress circumstances in his book, Why Torture
Doesn’t Work: The Neuroscience of Interrogation. While this may seem out of
place, O’Mara draws on findings from diverse areas, including the effects of stress,
sleep deprivation, anxiety, and other experiences that are common among clients
in and around the criminal-justice system. Taken together, all of these points
should raise concern among professionals.
As a final
point, it is worth noting that my colleagues, Jill Levenson, Gwenda Willis, and
I found that males
and females who have
sexually abused often have a higher rate of adverse childhood experiences in
their histories, raising further questions about the effects of their lives on
their memories.
It may be time
to acknowledge once and for all that, while we are quite certain that abuse
poses an unacceptable risk of harm to those who experience it, the research
shows how much noise there is in our systems as we try to retrieve and
understand the details. What’s missing (i.e. understanding the fluidity of
memory) from our understanding of clients matters, and sometimes the most
confident answer is that we don’t know the complete picture.
David Prescott, LISCW
Excellent post, David. Your movie example reminds me of the urban myth of "the spat-upon vet" returning home from the Vietnam War (see: bit.ly/spat-on). Many people cling to such false memories, impervious to all evidence of their falsity.
ReplyDelete